Chat with us, powered by LiveChat

Circulus in Demonstrando: The PNE Pain Deniers and Some Recent “Research”

POSTED ON IN Latest News BY Chris Centeno

PNE pain neuroscience education

There’s nothing like a good circular argument, what our esteemed Latin ancestors called circulus in demonstrando. You would think that scientific papers that focus on theories of why patients are disabled must be well grounded in objective measurement. After all, this is a topic that can determine whether or not some poor, injured soul gets to feed his family. However, for the pain neuroscience education crowd, circular reasoning has become a staple of the research house of cards on which the movement is built. A new paper in aboriginal peoples with low-back pain is a great example. Let me explain.

The Circular Argument Is a Trap That Has Been Plaguing Pain Researchers for Decades

Pain is tough to quantify, but in the past decade or so, we have actually found a few ways to measure it with advanced technology. However, historically, since injury and pain were difficult to measure, a classic pain research trap is to tie beliefs about disability to more physical disability and then conclude that the disability is caused by the beliefs. To show why this is a problem, let’s say our ability to diagnose what’s wrong with the complex systems in your car was limited. Let’s also say that you drive a “lemon” that sometimes starts in the morning and sometimes doesn’t start. You begin to develop the belief that you have, in fact, purchased a lemon. However, we could also say that since we have no way to tell if your car is, in fact, a lemon, that your beliefs about your car being a lemon have caused it to be a lemon.

If we suspend our thought experiment for a moment, we can examine the car to see if it’s a lemon or has significant issues in mechanical or electronic structure or function. If we did, we would see that the car is broken; hence, our logic connecting your beliefs to the car being a lemon as the reason it wouldn’t start are circular and seriously flawed. In conclusion, what breaks up the circular argument is the degree to which we are able to examine the car objectively.

Pain Studies That Don’t Include Complex and Objective Diagnostics Are Just like Our Lemon Study

While it’s easy to see the ridiculousness in the above study of a car that is believed to be a lemon, which, therefore, causes it to be a lemon, sometimes in pain studies, the best reviewers miss the obvious circular reasoning. Why? Because the causes of chronic pain are hard to diagnose and because most reviewers are frustrated by these patients themselves; hence, there is an inherent bias in believing that there is nothing wrong with these patients. However, our ability to diagnose pain has gotten better and better. In other words, our ability to objectively determine if a patient’s body is “a lemon” is improving.

Let’s take a simple study where we examine the beliefs of patients with chronic low-back pain by giving them questionnaires. We observe that the patients with the most pain and disability have the most significant beliefs about their own disability. For example, they score higher on questionnaires measuring thoughts and negative beliefs concerning disability. Hence, we conclude that it is the patients’ beliefs that cause their disability. As a corollary, we also conclude that we can change those beliefs through counseling, and, therefore, we can change their disability.

In our above study we, of course, are missing one thing: the ability to objectively tell if the patients who are the most disabled really are the most injured and, therefore, they have every right to have the most thoughts about their own disability and a pessimistic view about recovery. This is just like our car study, when we suddenly examined the car to determine if what we thought was a lemon really was mechanically a lemon.

So how could we do this study properly? It would be quite complex. First, we would have to perform extensive imaging of the low back, including using newer techniques to image the brain for pain responses (fMRI) and/or atrophy. Next, we would require double diagnostic blocks with blinded short- and long-acting anesthetic of all of the major structures of the low back to rule in or out that any one or more areas were causing pain. We would need movement-based low-back imaging looking at quantitative movement analysis. We would need multiple physicians to examine the patient and determine whether he or she was credible with regard to having a real chronic pain condition or was considered not credible. You get the idea. Doing a study like this right would be a very expensive affair, which is why 99% of these studies take the cheap and unethical way out—they skip all of this testing.

The New Aboriginal Pain Study

Now that you have an idea about how to construct a high-quality chronic-pain study looking at how beliefs can alter pain, let’s take a look at a study that was just published that was lauded by the PNE, chronic pain denier, set. Remember, this is a group of physical therapists who are trying hard to deny patients in chronic pain appropriate treatment by trying to convince them that their pain problem is really just hyperactive nerves and should be ignored.

The new study was performed with native aboriginal people in Australia. First, it’s very clear in the introduction of the paper that the authors have begun with a conclusion that beliefs about pain and structural changes in the spine define disability due to chronic low-back pain. They make absolutely no attempt to hide this bias:

“By influencing or perpetuating negative beliefs, there is the potential for healthcare practitioners to have a negative impact on their patients disorder. Negative perceptions about CLBP reflect widespread sociocultural attitudes in Western societies and are hypothesised to underlie the 20th century ‘epidemic’ of CLBP disability.”

Also interesting is that they claim that aboriginal peoples are “a population previously identified as protected against the disabling effects of low back pain due to cultural beliefs.” Reading the introduction confirms the origin for this statement is research that has shown that while aboriginal peoples do have low-back pain, they don’t seem as disabled by the phenomenon. This makes common sense as they live in a society where there are few options to treat the back pain and not working through the pain is not an option. We also read that other studies have actually shown the opposite, that aboriginal peoples are disabled by pain.

The researchers interviewed the subjects about their pain and beliefs and recorded and then transcribed their conversations. The researchers found that those subjects who described the most pain and disability also had the most descriptions about how some medical professional had told them about their low-back damage. That sounds about right, as the patients with the most disability would have been expected to see the most medical practitioners, would have been imaged the most, and would, therefore, recall more about what they had been told was wrong with their back. However, the researchers interpreted all of this to mean that the disability had been caused by the medical professionals who spoke with the patients.

The researchers also found that patients who had a more negative outlook on the future had the most pain. The researchers interpreted this as, the false perception of pain and damage (incorrect thoughts planted by evil healthcare practitioners) were influencing the patient’s outlook on life.

At no time did the researchers actually attempt to run all of the tests discussed above to determine if these patients did have physical problems that made them legitimately disabled. They didn’t try to find out what was wrong at all. What’s bizarre is that the researchers never even looked at the medical records of these patients, which would have been readily available in a country with a socialized medical care system.

How Did This Really Bad, Internally Conflicted Study Get Published?

How is it possible that a study that rests its conclusions on the idea that the subject’s spines aren’t really damaged never tried to determine if the patient’s spines were, in fact, damaged? How did this pass the most basic peer review? My guess would be that it was reviewed by other PNE pain deniers who fast-tracked it through the publication process.

As you can see above, the study is the worst case of circulus in demonstrando I’ve reviewed in a long time. Drawing conclusions about causation based on shoddy evidence. It’s also an example of reverse causation, or the concept that correlation doesn’t mean causation. For the latter, just because the level of disability and the belief about disability are correlated, doesn’t mean that one caused the other. Or in our car example, just because people with cars that are lemons believe more often that their car is, in fact, a lemon, doesn’t mean that believing your car is a lemon causes it to be a lemon!

The upshot? The PNE denier crowd loved this paper and for good reason as it gives a veneer of credibility to their claims that we healthcare providers disable patients. While I personally believe that there is some kernel of truth to the idea and that certain patients are more susceptible to this type of influence, the PNE chronic pain deniers try to take the argument way beyond its actual clinical importance to conflate their own significance.

 

    *DISCLAIMER: Like all medical procedures, Regenexx® Procedures have a success and failure rate. Patient reviews and testimonials on this site should not be interpreted as a statement on the effectiveness of our treatments for anyone else.
    Providers listed on the Regenexx website are for informational purposes only and are not a recommendation from Regenexx for a specific provider or a guarantee of the outcome of any treatment you receive.

    comments

    Sam says

    The beauty of these blogs is that not only they transfer valuable medical knowledge in simple terms but also they teach us how to process the (mis)information that are thrown at us with critical reasoning so that we do not take them for granted. There are very few out there who are true teachers in the sense that they care to teach what is right not what is profitable no matter what. Thanks for being a true teacher!

    replies

    Chris Centeno says

    Thanks Sam!

    replies

    Bob Piersanti says

    A very good analysis, but the deniers have always existed and published their work in many prestigious journals. Let's be honest, these journals are influenced by insurance companies and Big Pharma. Many of the papers are still a gold standard today and used extensively by insurance companies to deny medical coverage of those injured.
    I hope you would have published the citation of the new aboriginal study you wonderfully criticized.

    replies

    Chris Centeno says

    Bob,
    We are working on turning this into a publication. I have already published several articles on similar topics.

    replies

    Rebecca Gavin says

    I love learning more about how to spot logical fallacies and circular reasoning is one I have struggled with a bit. So I'm glad to hear you say it's hard to spot.

    I've been skeptical of the big push for cognitive behavioral treatment in conjunction with the way the pendulum has swung, once again, regarding the use of opioids. I recognize bias on my part, and my skepticism is based less on evidence or lack there of, and more on all of the phases I have seen pain management go through in the past 17 years, most of which were eventually disproved. For example, in 2000, the first pain doc I was referred to started me off on Fentanyl patches (primary care had been giving me Tylenol #3). That was absurd and kind of dangerous given what we've seen with the opioid addiction epidemic. I also remember when the new thing was to put everybody on long acting opioids based on the belief they were less prone to abuse and less likely to result in addiction. In my experience, any reluctance by the patient was suspicious. How did that theory work out? I have also experienced being treated like a junkie even when I wasn't even being seen for anything related to pain meds and wasn't asking for them. And I have seen the push now for psychological therapy to address just what you are describing - alleged beliefs and behaviors that are responsible for the patient's perception that their pain is disabling or unmanageable. Catastrophizing is a very popular shorthand now for blaming patients for their own pain. After 17 years (finally getting correct dx of Psoriatic Arthritis 3 years ago) I have coped with a lot of pain and to a certain level it's just background noise. So when my pain level is high enough to concern me, it's not because I'm over-reacting.

    Anyway, I appreciate being able to read a medically grounded response to this tiresome trend. I am always interested in having more tools for the old toolbox, but not when they're being shoved at me simultaneously with an attempt to take away a tool that I know works and use appropriately.

    Thanks

    replies

    Chris Centeno says

    Rebecca,

    Thanks so much for weighing in and sharing your perspective and experience. This is something we see time and time again, which is why this is such an important issue!

    replies

    Add comment

    Your comment will be revised by the site if needed.

    About the Author

    Chris Centeno

    Christopher J. Centeno, M.D. is an international expert and specialist in regenerative medicine and the clinical use of mesenchymal stem cells in orthopedics. He is board certified in physical medicine as well as rehabilitation and in pain management through The American Board of Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation.…

    View Profile

    Search Blog

    Categories